Skip to main content

Behavioral alterations induced by repeated saxitoxin exposure in drinking water

Abstract

Background

Blooms of the saxitoxin-producing cyanobacterium Cylindrospermopsis raciborskii have been contaminating drinking water reservoirs in Brazil for many years. Although acute effects of saxitoxin intoxication are well known, chronic deleterious outcomes caused by repeated saxitoxin exposure still require further investigation. The aim of the present work is to investigate the effects of consumption of drinking water contaminated with C. raciborskii for 30 days on learning and memory processes in rats.

Methods

The effects of saxitoxin (3 or 9 μg/L STX equivalents) or cyanobacteria on behavior was determined using the open field habituation task, elevated plus maze anxiety model task, inhibitory avoidance task, and referential Morris water maze task.

Results

No effects of saxitoxin consumption was observed on anxiety and motor exploratory parameters in the elevated plus maze and open field habituation tasks, respectively. However, groups treated with 9 μg/L STX equivalents displayed a decreased memory performance in the inhibitory avoidance and Morris water maze tasks.

Conclusions

These results suggest an amnesic effect of saxitoxin on aversive and spatial memories.

Background

Paralytic shellfish toxins (PST) constitute a group of neurotoxins produced by marine dinoflagellates and freshwater cyanobacteria, among them, saxitoxin (STX) represent the more frequent and toxic component. Overgrowth of dinoflagellates in the sea causes a toxic event commonly known as “red tide”, which is often associated with massive death of fish, bird, and other marine animals [1]. More than 30 structurally related naturally occurring PST molecules have been described, with all containing two guanidinium moieties [26]. Although its basic molecular skeleton comprises a 3,4,5-triakyl tetrahydropurine grouping, the toxin can be divided into the following three classes depending on the side-chain chemical structure: N-sulfocarbamoyl, decarbamoyl, and carbamate. The main species responsible for contamination in marine waters are three morphologically distinct genera of dinoflagellates (Alexandrium sp., Pyrodinium sp., and Gymnodinium sp.), while four different species of cyanobacteria (Anabaena circinalis, Aphanizomenon flos-aquae, Cylindrospermopsis raciborskii, and Lyngbya wollei) are the main species responsible in freshwaters [17].

An increase has been observed in the blooms of toxic STX-producing cyanobacteria in eutrophic water bodies worldwide [8, 9]. Cyanobacterial STX has attracted much scientific and public attention due to their toxicity and extensive geographic distribution [1015]. High concentrations of PST (1.57 μg/L STX equivalents) are detected in freshwater reservoirs in Brazil [16]. Therefore, the guideline concentration for STX in drinking water was described as 3.0 μg/L STX equivalents in the regulation number 2914/2011 of the Brazilian Ministry of Health. The same STX concentration was calculated and adopted as a health alert for STX equivalents in Australia [17]. However, there is no data concerning the toxicological effects of repeated consumption of contaminated drinking water at this STX concentration.

PST accumulates in marine shellfish and freshwater mussels at a very high concentration as a result of filter-feeding of dinoflagellates and cyanobacteria [18]. Subsequent human consumption of PST-contaminated shellfish and mussels commonly leads to the development of paralytic shellfish poisoning (PSP) with significant illness and mortality [6]. Symptoms of PSP intoxication include paresthesia of the mouth and lips, immobilization of the tongue, asthenia, dystonia, ataxia, dyspnea, nausea, vomiting, hypotension, tachycardia, and muscular and upper extremity weakness, followed by death in severe cases as a result of respiratory arrest [6, 19]. Relatively few studies have reported the poisoning of STX via water contamination [9].

Although acute effects of PST intoxication are well known, chronic deleterious effects on humans and other animals require further research. In addition to these diverse systemic effects, major toxic effects of STX are found in the brain [7]. STX is thought to be capable of crossing the blood–brain barrier (BBB), as it accumulates in the central nervous system (brain and medulla oblongata) [7, 20]. However, the transport mechanism through the BBB is still unknown.

The molecular action mechanism of STX is to block depolarization of excitable cells by reversibly binding to the voltage-gated sodium channels in the brain, peripheral nervous system, and muscles [4, 21]. The effects on other channels including HERG K1 channels and L-type calcium channels have also been reported [22, 23].

Although the primary molecular targets for STX in the CNS have been well studied, there is increasing evidence that the mode of action of PSPs extends beyond inhibition of neuronal transmission. It was demonstrated that exposure to STX from Aphanizomenon flos-aquae causes cell death in zebrafish brain tissue, with low doses inducing apoptosis and higher doses inducing necrosis [6]. Intraperitoneal (i.p.) administration of STX in some discrete rat brain regions has been reported to induce significant changes in quantities of neuroactive amino acids and serotonin [24]. However, i.p. administration of a sublethal dose of STX in rats resulted in acute alteration in dopamine production and its metabolite 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) [25]. These and many others neurophysiological aspects are important to modulate memory processes like formation and consolidation of long-term memory (LTM).

Memory formation process is susceptible to interference of many external agents and involves many biochemical and molecular events in specific brain regions, like hippocampus, amygdala and neocortex [26]. Moreover, the hippocampus has been identified as a major target site for triggering factors of damage, such as disease processes or neurotoxic exposure [27].

Tetrodotoxin (TTX) and lidocaine are widely used drugs in behavioral studies to transiently inactivate brain structures [28]. Both are specific voltage-gated sodium channels blockers, and thus prevent impulse generation and conduction, similar to the mechanism of STX [29]. Several reports have proved that hippocampal and amygdalar inactivation by TTX and lidocaine impairs several types of memories, including aversive contextual and spatial memories, but little is known about the effects of STX on cognition [28, 30, 31].

The present study investigated the effect of repeated exposure to water contaminated with PST-producing cyanobacteria at concentrations of 3 or 9 μg/L STX equivalents on learning and memory processes using rat memory behavioral models.

Methods

Source of toxins

Cells of C. raciborskii from the FCL/FURG culture collection were cultured in ASM-1 medium at 25 ± 1 °C and employed as a source of STX [32]. PST produced by C. raciborskii was detected by HPLC/FLD with a major (STX) and two minor peaks (neosaxitoxin and dc-saxitoxin) (Fig. 1), which were analyzed and compared with the commercial standards of STX, neosaxitoxin, and dc-saxitoxin (NRC, Canada). This strain was also examined for microcystin and cylindrospermopsin production by using of specific ELISA kits. No positive results were obtained within the kits detection range (0.05–2.5 μg/L).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Chromatogram showing PST from C. raciborskii used in the experiments. The toxin peak levels separated at 21.8, 32.2 and 37.7 min correspond to neosaxitoxin, dc-saxitoxin, and saxitoxin, respectively

A culture of the cyanobacteria Aphanothece sp. was used as a negative control for an unspecific cyanobacteria effect in the behavioral experiments. This strain of Aphanothece sp. did not produce toxins such as microcystin, cylindrospermopsin or PST and was cultured in BGN/2 medium (modified from the original BG11 composition by adding only half of the total nitrate composition) [33, 34].

Animals

Female Wistar rats (2 to 3 months old, weighting 210 to 300 g, n = 120) from our breeding facility were used. Animals were housed in plastic cages (five per cage) and maintained under a 12-h light/dark cycle and at a constant temperature of 24 ± 1 °C. The rats had access to water and food ad libitum.

Experiments with rats were performed in strict accordance with the Brazilian law and the Brazilian College of Animal Experimentation (COBEA), and were approved by the Ethics Committee of FURG (process number P025/2011 CEUA/FURG).

Experimental design

Experimental animals received water artificially contaminated with cyanobacterial cultures or its growth medium (control groups) for 30 days through drinking water bottles (800 mL). Five animals from each cage drank an average of 400 mL/day and no significant differences in consumption were observed among the experimental groups. Every week during the 30-day treatment period, the PST concentration (3 or 9 μg/L STX equivalents) in the drinking water was verified by HPLC/FLD. Each rat drank 0.24 or 0.72 μg of PST/day of 3 or 9 μg/L STX equivalents during treatment. PST concentrations in the bottles remained constant for at least a week. Only and when it was necessary, concentration correction to 3 or 9 μg/L was made with dilutions containing growth medium.

The effects of PST or Aphanothece sp. on behavior was determined immediately after the 30-day treatment period using the open field habituation task (OFH), the elevated plus maze anxiety (EPM) task, the inhibitory avoidance (IA) task, and the Morris water maze (MWM) task [3538].

The experimental design was composed of five groups: (1) rats that received BGN/2 medium (control group for Aphanothece sp.), (2) rats that received Aphanothece sp. (a non-toxic culture), (3) rats that received ASM-1 medium (control group for C. raciborskii), (4) rats that received 3 μg/L STX equivalents from PST-producing C. raciborskii culture, and (5) rats that received 9 μg/L STX equivalents from PST-producing C. raciborskii. Solutions containing the abovementioned five media were renewed daily. Toxin concentrations were also verified every week by HPLC/FLD.

Behavioral procedures

The OFH task was conducted using a 50 cm high, 60 cm × 40 cm plywood box with a frontal glass wall and a linoleum floor divided in 12 equal rectangles. Animals were left for 5 min both in the training and test sessions, and the number of rearings and crossings between sectors were registered. The difference between the two sessions in the number of rearings and of crossings between rectangles was considered a measure of habituation to the open field. If the animals habituated to the field during the first session, they should recognize it as familiar, and consequently, the number of rearings and crossings should decrease in the second session. The number of crossings in the test session was used as a control for the possible motor and general performance effects of the treatments [35].

The EPM task was conducted using wooden arms that were 50 cm long and 10 cm wide. The apparatus was elevated at a height of 70 cm above the floor. The closed arms were surrounded by a 15 cm wall. Environmental temperature was maintained at that measured in the housing room. The testing room was illuminated with a dim white light that provided 100 lux for the open arms and 50 lux for the closed ones. Each animal, experimentally naive, was placed in the central platform of EPM, facing an open arm, and allowed to freely explore for 5 min. The time spent in the open arms and in the closed arms was registered. After each observation, the apparatus was cleaned with ethyl alcohol (10 %) to remove organic residues left from the preceding animal. Experiments were recorded using a video camera and video files were stored in a personal computer for analyses. The time spent at each open or closed arm was the anxiety index of the task. Rats prefer to stay at hidden places, and thus, they spent more time at the closed arms [36].

The IA task was conducted in an automatically operated and brightly illuminated box in which the left extreme of the grid (42.0 cm × 25.0 cm grid of parallel 0.1 cm caliber stainless steel bars spaced 1.0 cm apart) was covered by a 7.0 cm wide and 5.0 cm high Formica® laminate covered platform. Animals were placed on the platform and their latency to step down with their four paws on the grid was measured. In the training session, immediately upon stepping down, the animals received a 0.5 mA, 3.0 s scrambled footshock. In the test session, no footshock was given and a ceiling of 180 s was imposed to the step-down latency. The latency to step down to the platform in the test session was the aversive memory index for the task [37, 3942].

The water maze was a black circular pool (190 cm in diameter) conceptually divided into four equal imaginary quadrants for the intention of data analysis. The water temperature was 21–23 °C. Two centimeters below the surface of the water was a black circular platform (12 cm in diameter) hidden from the rats view. It had a rough surface, which allowed the rat to climb onto it easily once its presence was detected. The swimming path of the rats was recorded, in the test session, using a video camera mounted above the center of the pool and analyzed using a video tracking and analysis system. The water maze was located in a white room with several visual stimuli leaned on the walls to provide spatial cues. Rats were handled 5 min per day for three days prior to training. Training in the hidden platform (spatial) version of MWM was conducted during four consecutive days. On each day, rats received four consecutive training trials during which the hidden platform was kept at a constant location. A different starting location was used on each trial, which consisted of swimming followed by sitting during 30 s on the platform. Any rat that did not find the platform within 60 s (time of each trial) was guided to it by the experimenter. The interval between trials was 30 s. During each interval, rats were carefully dried with a towel. Memory retention was evaluated in a 60-s test probe conducted in the absence of the escape platform 24 h after the last training session. In the test probe of MWM, the time spent at the target platform quadrant (TPQ) and at the opposite platform quadrant (OPQ) was registered. The memory retention of the MWM was observed in the probe test session when rats spend more time in TPQ than in OPQ [35, 43].

Statistical analysis was conducted using the data of the behavioral models and included number of rearings and crossings in OFH, latencies to step down in IA, time spent in the close and open arms of EPM, time to arrive at the ridden platform in the trainings of MWM. All data were evaluated by SPSS 18.0 for Windows.

Since the step-down latencies have not passed a normality test (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test with Lilliefors’ correction), differences among groups were evaluated by Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA with Dunn’s all pair-wise multiple comparison post-hoc test; training versus test latencies were correspondingly compared by the Wilcoxon signed ranks test. In the OFH, EPM and MWM tasks, as in their respective deviations, variables were normally distributed. Groups were compared by Student’s t-test or ANOVA. In the MWM task, the learning phase was evaluated using repeated measures ANOVA. Significance was set at p < 0.05.

Results

There were no clinical signs of dysfunction in the treated rats throughout the experimental period nor were there any significant deviations in body weight gain in both cyanobacteria treated rats, compared with the controls.

In the OFH (Fig. 2) task, rats received water artificially contaminated with PST-producing C. raciborskii for 30 days. None of the experimental groups showed significant differences in the number of rearings or crossings neither in the training nor in the test sessions (p > 0.05, one-way ANOVA test). Both variables (rearings and crossings) were significantly lower in the test than in the training session for all groups (p < 0.05, ANOVA test), indicating no mnemonic effects on this task. The fact that there were no observed differences in behavior among groups suggests that neither locomotor activity nor exploratory effects have been caused by the chronic consumption of STX-contaminated water (3 or 9 μg/L STX equivalents) or Aphanothece sp.

Fig. 2
figure 2

a and b Absence of effect of all treatments in the OFH task. Data expressed as mean ± SEM; n = 10 per group. a Number of crossings and rearings in the test probe are significantly different from the corresponding training values for all groups (ANOVA test, p < 0.05)

The EPM task (Fig. 3) is an anxiety model. In this experiment, two experimental groups received treatment for 30 days: a control group (ASM1 medium without cyanobacteria) and a PST-producing C. raciborskii (9 μg/L STX equivalents) group. Compared to the control group, the STX-treated rats did not show a significant difference in the time spent in the open or closed arms of the EPM apparatus (p > 0.05 for both variables, Student’s t-test). These results indicated that neither anxiogenic nor anxiolitic effects were observed by the chronic consumption of PST-contaminated water (9 μg/L STX equivalents).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Absence of effect of PST-producing C. raciborskii (9 μg/L STX equivalents) in the EPM task. Data expressed as mean ± SEM; n = 10 per group; TSCA: time spent at closed arms; TSOA: time spent at open arms; TSCA: time spent at central area

The IA task was a contextual fear conditioning memory model and results are shown in Fig. 4. Five experimental groups were submitted to the test as previously mentioned in the Materials and Methods session. As data were not normally distributed (Kolmogorov-Smirnov normality test, p > 0.200), non-parametric tests were used. Only the highest PST concentration (9 μg/L STX equivalents) in drinking water impaired the performance; rats from this group significantly spent lower time at the platform in the test session when compared to the other groups (p < 0.05, Dunn’s all pair-wise multiple comparison post-hoc test, after a Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA with p = 0.011). Groups were comparable because there were no significant differences among training session latencies (p = 0.229, Kruskal-Wallis ANOVA). However, all groups have displayed IA task learning, as each test latency was significantly larger than the corresponding training one (p < 0.005 for all experimental groups, Wilcoxon signed-ranks test). These results suggested an amnesic effect of the highest concentration (9 μg/L STX equivalents) of long lasting consumption of PST in drinking water.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Effect of consumption of cyanobacteria-contaminated drinking water in the step-down IA task. Data expressed as median and interquartile intervals (training session in white; test session in gray); n = 10 rats per group. Kruskal-Wallis test shows no significant difference among training session latencies (p > 0.05). a Each of the five experimental groups has shown a significant difference between training and test session latencies (p < 0.05, Wilcoxon test). b Only the PST-producing C. raciborskii (9 μg/L STX equivalents) group showed a significant difference in the test session latency compared to the control group (p < 0.05, Dunn test)

The reference spatial memory model of the MWM task results are shown in Figs. 5, 6 and 7. Two experimental groups were applied: control group (ASM1 medium without cyanobacteria) and PST-producing C. raciborskii (9 μg/L STX equivalents).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Effect of consumption of cyanobacteria-contaminated drinking water in the training trial of MWM task. Data expressed as mean ± SEM; n = 15 rats per group. Both groups showed significant reduction in latency to find the platform during the four training days (p < 0.05, repeated measure ANOVA). The PST-producing C. raciborskii (9 μg/L STX equivalents) group showed a significant difference compared to the control group (p < 0.05, repeated measure ANOVA)

Fig. 6
figure 6

Effect of consumption of cyanobacteria-contaminated drinking water in the test session of MWM task. Data expressed as mean ± SEM; n = 15 rats per group. a Only the ASM-1 control group showed significant difference between time spent at TPQ and time spent at OPQ, p < 0.05 (one-way ANOVA test). b Rats treated with PST-producing C. raciborskii (9 μg/L STX equivalents) showed significant difference in the time spent at TPQ (p = 0.001, one-way ANOVA test and Tukey post-hoc test)

Fig. 7
figure 7

Absence of effect of PST-producing C. raciborskii (9 μg/L STX equivalents) on the swimming path length during the test session of the MWM task (p = 0.431, one-way ANOVA test). Data expressed as mean ± SEM; n = 15 per group

At the end of the four consecutive training days (Fig. 5), the animals treated with PST took a significantly longer time to find the hidden platform than the control group (p = 0.024, repeated measures ANOVA). However, both groups demonstrated a significant latency reduction to find the platform over the four training days (p < 0.05, repeated measures ANOVA), indicating learning in both groups.

In the probe test session of the MWM task (Fig. 6), the PST-treated group of rats significantly spent less time at the TPQ than the control group (p = 0.001, one-way ANOVA test and Tukey post-hoc test). Moreover, the PST-treated group did not show any difference in the time spent at the TPQ compared with the time spent at the OPQ (p = 0.218, repeated measures ANOVA). In the test session, there was no significant difference in the path length (Fig. 7) between groups (p = 0.431, one-way ANOVA test). Moreover, there was no significant difference between groups concerning swimming speed (p = 0.387, one-way ANOVA test), which indicated no motor effects of STX on MWM.

These results suggest an evident amnesic effect of consumption PST-producing C. raciborskii in drinking water (9 μg/L STX equivalents) on the spatial reference memory of the MWM task and on the aversive memory of the IA task.

Discussion

Behavioral alteration could be a reflection of environmental perturbation [44]. Since behavior associates physiological function with ecological processes for a given species, it might provide a useful indicator or biomarker for detecting harmful chemical pollutants [45].

Behavioral changes are a result of a complex modulation in the biochemistry and physiological lower organization levels. The knowledge concerning these fundamental processes is important to understand how an organism would adjust its responses to environmental stimuli. Moreover, adaptation to the environment is a neuronal signal transduction-dependent process in response to biochemistry and genetic expression changes [46].

This study showed results of the effects of long lasting consumption of PST-contaminated drinking water on behavioral parameters and correlated an STX decrease effect to learning and memory processes. In our behavioral study, there was an evident amnesic effect of chronic PST (9.0 μg/L STX equivalents) administration through artificially contaminated drinking water. Memory impairment occurred in both tasks that present different cognitive components: the aversive memory of the IA task, which is dependent on stress modulation (glucocorticoids and noradrenergic activation) [26, 47], and the reference spatial memory of the MWM task [37, 43]. Both memory tasks depend on neurotransmitter system components (glutamatergic, gabaergic and cholinergic), molecular pathways (second messengers and kinases proteins), and brain regions (hippocampal formation, amygdaloid complex and neocortex) related with declarative memory formation in humans [46, 48, 49].

However, no effect was found in the less aversive exploratory OFH task (Fig. 2). The unaltered number of crossings in the OFH task and the effective concentration in the IA and MWM task supports the idea that the effect of PST in the IA and MWM tasks is basically cognitive and not a motor or an exploratory effect [35]. Moreover, in the EPM task, no effect of 9 μg/L STX equivalents was observed, indicating either anxiolytic or anxiogenic status caused by PST [36].

Our non-toxic culture control treatment did not show any effect in OFH and IA tasks. These results supported the idea of a mnemonic effect of PST produced by C. raciborskii. However, nonspecific synergy effects caused by secondary metabolites may also contribute with STX in the memory impairment observed.

Furthermore, no effects were observed in the groups treated with 3 μg/L STX equivalents. This result supports the STX concentration guideline in drinking water present in the regulation number 2914/2011 of the Brazilian Ministry of Health. However, recent work reported cytotoxicity, genotoxicity and oxidative stress caused by 3 μg/L of STX in teleost neurons in vitro [50].

Voltage-gated sodium channels, blocked by STX, are expressed for all regions of the vertebrate brain [4, 51]. These channels positively modulate the mechanisms of synaptic plasticity processes and induce long-term potentiation (LTP, memory electrophysiological model) at hippocampus [52, 53]. Our results suggest that STX-sensitive voltage-gated sodium channels positively modulate the learning and memory processes of the IA and MWM tasks.

Co-expression of STX-sensitive voltage-gated sodium channel and receptor tyrosine kinase TrkB at the same places on neurons was reported [51, 53]. Binding of the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) to the TrkB receptors co-activates the voltage-gated sodium channel producing rapid neuronal depolarization that induces LTP [51, 53]. Moreover, STX administration over neurons blocks voltage-gated sodium channel and inhibits LTP induced by BDNF [51]. Neurotrophins like BDNF are regulatory factors that mediate differentiation and survival of neurons in the peripheral and central nervous systems and positively modulate learning and memory processes [54].

It was reported that fish exposed to transport stress in the presence of STX displayed significant reduction in neural c-Fos expression [55]. The immediately early gene c-fos, and its protein c-Fos, is known to be induced in neurons of mammals and fish as a result of neuronal stimulation. Moreover, in rats it has been demonstrated that c-Fos expression mediates neuronal excitation and enhances survival, processes associated with memory consolidation [56].

The studies demonstrated that STX impaired memory. The toxic mechanisms of STX on memory processes must be due to damage of the molecular mechanisms involved with control of synaptic plasticity like signaling pathways of BDNF and c-Fos.

However, STX must be involved with oxidative stress and cellular death in some brain regions like hippocampus or neocotex [50, 57]. Recently, it were reported that the consumption of water contaminated with cyanobacteria PSP-producing Cylindrospermopsis raciborskii for 30 days caused significant alterations in the antioxidant machinery, both in the brain structures such as the hippocampal formation and the prefrontal cortex as well as in the liver of exposed rats [58]. Thus, memory impairment observed in this work can to be consequence of oxidative stress at hippocampus and prefrontal cortex of rats repeated exposed to saxitoxin-contaminated drinking water.

Conclusions

In summary, the repeated exposure of rats to contaminated drinking water with C. raciborskii producer of PST for 30 days caused deleterious behavioral alterations including memory impairment in inhibitory avoidance task and referential water maze task. The neurotoxic effect of saxitoxin or variants and possible nonspecific damages caused by C. raciborskii bloom are problems that require further investigation in order to mitigate a possible impact on the environment.

Ethics approval

Animal tests were performed in accordance with the guidelines of the Brazilian College of Animal Experimentation (COBEA), and were approved by the Ethics Committee of FURG (process number P025/2011 CEUA/FURG).

References

  1. Barthram J, Carmichael WW, Jones G, Skulberg OM. Introduction. In: Chorus I, Bartram J, Bartram J, editors. Toxic cyanobacteria in water: a guide to their public health consequences, monitoring and management. London: WHO, E. & FN Spon; 1999. p. 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Oshima Y, Blackburn SI, Hallegraeff GM. Comparative study on paralytic shellfish toxin profiles of the dinoflagellate Gymnodinium catenatum from three different countries. Mar Biol. 1993;116:471–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Onodera H, Satake M, Oshima Y. New saxitoxin analogues from the freshwater filamentous cyanobacterium Lyngbya wollei. Nat Toxins. 1997;5(4):146–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Llewellin LE. Saxitoxin, a toxic marine natural product that targets a multitude of receptors. Nat Prod Rep. 2006;23(2):200–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Aráoz R, Molgó J, Tandeau de Marsac N. Neurotoxic cyanobacterial toxins. Toxicon. 2010;56(5):813–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Zhang D, Hu C, Wang G, Li D, Li G, Liu Y. Zebrafish neurotoxicity from aphantoxins-cyanobacterial paralytic shellfish poisons (PSPs) from Aphanizomenon flos-aquae DC-1. Environ Toxicol. 2013;28(5):239–54. doi:10.1002/tox.20714.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Cervantes Cianca RC, Pallares MA, Durán Barbosa R, Vidal Adan L, Martins JML, Gago-Martínez A. Application of precolumn oxidation HPLC method with fluorescence detection to evaluate saxitoxin levels in discrete brain regions of rats. Toxicon. 2007;49(1):89–99.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Carmichael WW. The toxins of cyanobacteria. Sci Am. 1994;270(1):78–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Falconer IR. Overview of problems caused by toxic blue-green algae (cyanobacteria) in drinking and recreational water. Environ Toxicol. 1999;14(1):5–12.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Lagos N, Onodera H, Zagatto PA, Andrinolo D, Azevedo SM, Oshima Y. The first evidence of paralytic shellfish toxins in the freshwater cyanobacterium Cylindrospermopsis raciborskii, isolated from Brazil. Toxicon. 1999;37(10):1359–73.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Pomati F, Sacchi S, Rossetti C, Giovannardi S, Onodera H, Oshima Y, et al. The freshwater cyanobacterium Planktothrix sp. FP1: molecular identification and detection of paralytic shellfish poisoning toxins. J Phycol. 2000;36(3):553–62.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Liu YM, Chen W, Li DH, Shen YW, Li GB, Liu YD. First report of aphantoxins in China-waterblooms of toxigenic Aphanizomenon flos-aquae in Lake Dianchi. Ecotoxicol Environ Saf. 2006;65(1):84–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Ballot A, Fastner J, Wiedner C. Paralytic shellfish poisoning toxin-producing cyanobacterium Aphanizomenon gracile in northeast Germany. Appl Environ Microbiol. 2010;76(4):1173–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Ledreux A, Thomazeau S, Catherine A, Duval C, Yéprémian C, Marie A, et al. Evidence for saxitoxins production by the cyanobacterium Aphanizomenon gracile in a French recreational water body. Harmful Algae. 2010;10(1):88–97.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Clemente Z, Busato RH, Ribeiro CAO, Cestari MM, Ramsdorf WA, Magalhães VF, et al. Analyses of paralytic shellfish toxin and biomarkers in a southern Brazilian reservoir. Toxicon. 2010;55(2-3):396–406.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Yunes JS, Cunha NT, Barros LP, Proença LA, Monserrat JM. Cyanobacterial neurotoxins from Southern Brazilian freshwaters. Comment Toxicol. 2003;9:103–15.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Fitzgerald DJ, Cunliffe DA, Burch MD. Development of health alerts for cyanobacteria and related toxins in drinking water in South Australia. Environ Toxicol. 1999;14(1):203–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Nogueira ICG, Pereira P, Dias E, Pflugmacher S, Wiegand C, Franca S, et al. Accumulation of paralytic shellfish toxins (PST) from the cyanobacterium Aphanizomenon issatschenkoi by the cladoceran Daphnia magna. Toxicon. 2004;44(7):773–80.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. David LS, Nicholson RA. Quantitation of paralytic shellfish toxins using mouse brain synaptoneurosomes. Chemosphere. 2004;55(10):1315–21.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Andrinolo D, Michea LF, Lagos N. Toxic effects, pharmacokinetics and clearance of saxitoxin, a component of paralytic shellfish poison (PSP), in cats. Toxicon. 1999;37(3):447–64.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Andrinolo D, Iglesias V, Garcia C, Lagos N. Toxicokinetics and toxicodynamics of gonyautoxins after an oral toxin dose in cats. Toxicon. 2002;40(6):699–709.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Wang JX, Salata JJ, Bennett PB. Saxitoxin is a gating modifier of HERG K+ Channels. J Gen Physiol. 2003;121(6):583–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Su Z, Sheets M, Ishida H, Li F, Barry WH. Saxitoxin blocks L-type ICa. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2004;308(1):324–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Cervantes CR, Durán R, Faro LF, Alfonso PM. Effects of systemic administration of saxitoxin on serotonin levels in some discrete rat brain regions. Med Chem. 2009;5(4):336–42.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Cervantes CR, Faro LF, Durán BR, Alfonso PM. Alterations of 3,4-dihydroxyphenylethylamine and its metabolite 3,4-dihydroxyphenylacetic produced in rat brain tissues after systemic administration of saxitoxin. Neurochem Int. 2011;59(5):643–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Izquierdo I, Medina JH. Memory formation: the sequence of biochemical events in the hippocampus and its connection to activity in other brain structures. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1997;68(3):285–316.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Harry GJ, d’Hellencourt CL. Dentate gyrus: alterations that occur with hippocampal injury. Neurotoxicology. 2003;24(3):343–56.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Cimadevilla JM, Lópes F, Nieto L, Aguirre MJ, Fernandez R. Lidocaine, tetrodotoxin and their effect on consolidation of spatial memory. Psicothema. 2009;21(3):471–4.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Stevens M, Peigneur S, Tytgat J. Neurotoxins and their binding areas on voltage-gated sodium channels. Front Pharmacol. 2011;2:71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Vafaei AA, Jesek K, Bures J, Fenton AA, Rashidy-Pour A. Post-training reversible inactivation of the rat’s basolateral amygdala interferes with hippocampus-dependent place avoidance memory in a time-dependent manner. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2007;88(1):87–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Telensky P, Svoboda J, Blahna K, Bures J, Kubik S, Stuchlik A. Functional inactivation of the rat hippocampus disrupts avoidance of a moving object. Proc Natl Acad Sci Unit States Am. 2011;108(13):5414–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Gorham PR, McLachlan J, Hammer UT, Kim WK. Isolation and culture of toxic strains of Anabaena flos-aquae (Lyngb.) de Bréd. Verh Internat Verein Limnol. 1964;15:796–804.

    Google Scholar 

  33. Rippka R, Deruelles J, Waterbury JB, Herdman M, Stanier RY. Generic assignments, strain histories, and properties of pure cultures of cyanobacteria. J Gen Microbiol. 1979;111:1–61.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Leão JC, Geracitano LA, Monserrat JM, Amado LL, Yunes JS. Microcystin-induced oxidative stress in Laeonereis acuta (Polychaeta, Nereididae). Mar Environ Res. 2008;66(1):92–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Diehl F, Oliveira LF, Sánchez G, Camboim C, de Oliveira Alvares L, Lanziotti VB, et al. Facilitatory effect of the intra-hippocampal pre-test administration of MT3 in the inhibitory avoidance task. Behav Brain Res. 2007;177(2):227–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Carobrez AP, Bertoglio LJ. Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29(8):1193–205.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Izquierdo I, Barros DM, Mello e Souza T, de Souza MM, Izquierdo LA, Medina JH. Mechanims for memory types differ. Nature. 1998;393(6686):635–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Aguiar RB, Dickel OE, Cunha RW, Monserrat JM, Barros DM, Martinez PE. Estradiol valerate and tibolone: effects on memory. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2006;85(4):689–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Barros DM, Mello e Souza T, De David T, Choi H, Aguzzoli A, Madche C, et al. Simultaneous modulation of retrieval by dopaminergic D1, b-noradrenergic, serotoninergic-1A and cholinergic muscarinic receptors in cortical structures of the rat. Behav Brain Res. 2001;124(1):1–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Barros DM, Galhardi FG, Ribas Ferreira JL, Guterres LB, Dickel O, Geracitano LA, et al. The benefits and drawbacks of nicotine exposure in the cortex and hippocampus of old rats. Neurotoxicology. 2007;28(3):562–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Gonçalves CR, Cunha CW, Barros DM, Martínez PE. Effects of prenatal and postnatal exposure to a low dose of bisphenol A on behavior and memory in rats. Environ Toxicol Pharmacol. 2010;30(2):195–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Parfitt GM, Campos CR, Barbosa AK, Koth AP, Barros DM. Participation of hippocampal cholinergic system in memory persistence for inhibitory avoidance in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2012;97(2):183–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Rossato JI, Bevilaqua LR, Medina JH, Izquierdo I, Cammarota M. Retrieval induces hippocampal-dependent reconsolidation of spatial memory. Learn Mem. 2006;13(4):431–40.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Zala SM, Penn DJ. Abnormal behaviours induced by chemical pollution: a review of the evidence and new challenges. Anim Behav. 2004;68(4):649–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Kist LW, Piato AL, Santos da Rosa JG, Koakoski G, Barcellos LJG, Yunes JS, et al. Acute exposure to microcystin-producing cyanobacterium Microcystis aeruginosa alters adult zebrafish (Danio rerio) swimming performance parameters. J Toxicol. 2011;2011:280304. 9 pages.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Kandel ER, Kupfermann I, Iversen S. Learning and memory. In: Kandel ER, Schuartz JH, Jessell TM, editors. Principles of neural science. 4th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill; 2000. p. 1227–47.

    Google Scholar 

  47. McGaugh JL, Roozendaal B. Role of adrenal stress hormones in forming lasting memories in the brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12(2):205–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Squire LR. Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from finding with rats, monkeys and humans. Psychol Rev. 1992;99(2):195–231.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Roozendaal B, McGaugh JL. Memory modulation. Behav Neurosci. 2011;125(6):797–824.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. da Silva CA, de Morais EC, Costa MDM, Ribas JLC, Guiloski IC, Ramsdorf WA, et al. Saxitoxins induce cytotoxicity, genotoxicity and oxidative stress in teleost neurons in vitro. Toxicon. 2014;86:8–15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Blum R, Kafitz KW, Konnerth A. Neurotrophin-evoked depolarization requires the sodium channel Na(V)1.9. Nature. 2002;419(6908):687–93.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Bliss TV, Collingridge GL. A synaptic model of memory: long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature. 1993;361(6407):31–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Rose CR, Blum R, Kafitz KW, Kovalchuk Y, Konnerth A. From modulator to mediator: rapid effects of BDNF on ion channels. Bioessays. 2004;26(11):1185–94.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Bekinschtein P, Cammarota M, Katche C, Slipczuk L, Rossato JI, Goldin A, et al. BDNF is essential to promote persistence of long-term memory storage. Proc Natl Acad Sci Unit States Am. 2008;105(7):2711–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Salierno JD, Snyder NS, Murphy AZ, Poli M, Hall S, Baden D, et al. Harmful algal bloom toxins alter c-Fos protein expression in the brain of killifish, Fundulus heteroclitus. Aquat Toxicol. 2006;78(4):350–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Radulovic J, Tronson NC. Molecular specificity of multiple hippocampal processes governing fear extinction. Rev Neurosci. 2010;21(1):1–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. da Silva CA, Oba ET, Ramsdorf WA, Magalhães VF, Cestari MM, Ribeiro CAO, et al. First report about saxitoxins in freshwater fish Hoplias malabaricus through trophic exposure. Toxicon. 2011;57(1):141–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Ramos PB, Diehl F, dos Santos JM, Monserrat JM, Yunes JS. Oxidative stress in rats induced by consumption of saxitoxin contaminated drink water. Harmful Algae. 2014;37:68–74.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Luiz Carlos Silveira Costa for his helpful assistance with animal maintenance.

Funding

This research was supported by fellowships and grants from the Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES – PRODOC) and the National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq – MCT). JSY and DMB are research fellows of CNPq.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Felipe Diehl.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

FD wrote the first draft of the manuscript and revised it; he also performed all behavioral experiments. PBR contributed to experimental design and animal maintenance. JMS performed all chromatographic analysis. DMB and JSY helped in designing all the experiments and corrected the final version of article. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Diehl, F., Ramos, P.B., dos Santos, J.M. et al. Behavioral alterations induced by repeated saxitoxin exposure in drinking water. J Venom Anim Toxins Incl Trop Dis 22, 18 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40409-016-0072-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40409-016-0072-9

Keywords